Jump to content

Perception: Difference between revisions

m
no edit summary
mNo edit summary
mNo edit summary
Line 12: Line 12:


====Visual perception====
====Visual perception====
Visual perception can be understood as a diverse process that begins with the transformation of light stimuli into meaningful cognitive interpretations involving retinal sensing through photoreceptors and cortical processing in multiple brain areas.<ref name=":0">Donato, R., Pavan, A., & Campana, G. (2020). Investigating the Interaction Between Form and Motion Processing: A Review of Basic Research and Clinical Evidence. ''Frontiers in Psychology'', ''11''. <nowiki>https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.566848</nowiki></ref> The photoreceptors receive light signals through the retina, converting them into electrical signals. Those signals are then transmitted along the optic nerve in the eye, in order to reach the lateral geniculate nucleus before arriving at the striate cortex. This cortex, known as the visual cortex serves on fundamental basis for the conscious perception of static form and local brightness differences, establishing the base for more complex visual processing.<ref>Pollen, D. A. (1999). On the Neural Correlates of Visual Perception. ''Cerebral Cortex'', ''9''(1), 4–19. <nowiki>https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/9.1.4</nowiki></ref> Following that, after leaving the visual cortex, signals travel along the dorsal stream to the parietal cortex, serving for spatial orientation and motor actions such as reaching or eye movements. Further, focusing on forms, colours and object identity, signals must flow through the ventral stream into the inferior temporal cortex.<ref name=":0" /> For perceptual experience to be created, the visual cortex engages in recursive feedback loops with higher brain regions, for instance temporal and parietal. Those feedbacks enter into loops between each other to continuously compare new sensory data with prior knowledge or expectations, leading to our visual recognition of the outer world.<ref name=":0" /> Visual perception involves actively searching for relevant stimuli, influenced by external factors such as color salience and movement, as well as internal states in order to recognise objects. For instance, conspicuous features can capture human attention instantly, leading to unusual preferences when distractions occur. At this state, the ventral stream capture specific details from it. In addition, the temporal cortex stores those representations, helping humans to categorise and label familiar objects in fractions of a second. <ref name=":1">Jansson-Boyd, C. V., & Bright, P. (2024). Visual neuroscience. ''Elsevier EBooks'', 51–69. <nowiki>https://doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-443-13581-1.00004-2</nowiki></ref> Furthermore, visual search engages emotional and reward circuits, when identifying form and motion. The ventral tegmental area and nucleus accumbens interact with cortical regions to process rewarding stimuli, reinforcing behaviour patterns triggered by appealing elements. Likewise research shows, that emotional associations are carried firmly throughout visual perception, that bias us towards or against objects before consciously registering the object. This phenomenon, known as microvalence, refers to subconscious evaluation of an object's aversiveness during visual processing.<ref name=":1" />
Visual perception can be understood as a diverse process that begins with the transformation of light stimuli into meaningful cognitive interpretations involving retinal sensing through photoreceptors and cortical processing in multiple brain areas.<ref name=":0">Donato, R., Pavan, A., & Campana, G. (2020). Investigating the Interaction Between Form and Motion Processing: A Review of Basic Research and Clinical Evidence. ''Frontiers in Psychology'', ''11''. <nowiki>https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.566848</nowiki></ref> The photoreceptors receive light signals through the retina, converting them into electrical signals. Those signals are then transmitted along the optic nerve in the eye, in order to reach the lateral geniculate nucleus before arriving at the striate cortex. This cortex, known as the visual cortex serves on fundamental basis for the conscious perception of static form and local brightness differences, establishing the base for more complex visual processing.<ref>Pollen, D. A. (1999). On the Neural Correlates of Visual Perception. ''Cerebral Cortex'', ''9''(1), 4–19. <nowiki>https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/9.1.4</nowiki></ref> Following that, after leaving the visual cortex, signals travel along the dorsal stream to the parietal cortex, serving for spatial orientation and motor actions such as reaching or eye movements. Further, focusing on forms, colours and object identity, signals must flow through the ventral stream into the inferior temporal cortex.<ref name=":0" /> For perceptual experience to be created, the visual cortex engages in recursive feedback loops with higher brain regions, for instance temporal and parietal. Those feedbacks enter into loops between each other to continuously compare new sensory data with prior knowledge or expectations, leading to our visual recognition of the outer world.<ref name=":0" /> Visual perception involves actively searching for relevant stimuli, influenced by external factors such as color salience and movement, as well as internal states in order to recognise objects. For instance, conspicuous features can capture human attention instantly, leading to unusual preferences when distractions occur. At this state, the ventral stream captures specific details from it. In addition, the temporal cortex stores those representations, helping humans to categorise and label familiar objects in fractions of a second. <ref name=":1">Jansson-Boyd, C. V., & Bright, P. (2024). Visual neuroscience. ''Elsevier EBooks'', 51–69. <nowiki>https://doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-443-13581-1.00004-2</nowiki></ref> Furthermore, visual search engages emotional and reward circuits, when identifying form and motion. The ventral tegmental area and nucleus accumbens interact with cortical regions to process rewarding stimuli, reinforcing behaviour patterns triggered by appealing elements. Likewise research shows, that emotional associations are carried firmly throughout visual perception, that bias us towards or against objects before consciously registering the object. This phenomenon, known as microvalence, refers to subconscious evaluation of an object's aversiveness during visual processing.<ref name=":1" />


====Perception of touch ====
====Haptic system ====
Within the skin, the essential, external organ for touch, exist sensory cells, that react to differences in pressure and temperature. The information recorded is sent to the brain and interpreted there.
Touch helps us to navigate through the physical space, by integrating information from the skin, muscles and joints to foster a cohesive perception of objects, surfaces and spatial relationships. While joints and muscles enables feedback about cup's orientation and weight, the skin contains a complex system of specialised nerve endings designed to detect mechanical stimuli such as pressure, vibration, and texture. <ref name=":02">Reed, Catherine L., and Mounia Ziat. “Haptic Perception: From the Skin to the Brain ☆.” ''Reference Module in Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Psychology'', 2018, <nowiki>https://doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-809324-5.03182-5</nowiki>.</ref> The majority of receptors, which are distributed throughout the layers of the skin, are represented by:


=====Haptic perception=====
# Merkel's Disc's:
Haptic perception regards the exploration of surfaces and objects by a moving subject in opposition to passive contact by a static subject<ref name="ref3"/>.
#* Located in the deep epidermis
The term „Haptik“ was first introduced by Max Dessoir for a term of the research of the sense of touch. Later Gibson defined it as "[t]he sensibility of the individual to the world adjacent to his body by use of his body“<ref name="ref4"/>.
#* Highly sensitive to light touch and fine details
For identification of three dimensional objects four exploratory procedures have been identified
# Hair Follicle Receptors:
* Lateral motion
#* Responding to bending or subtle movement of hair
* Pressure
#* Detection of gentle stimuli (e.g. a soft breeze)
* Enclosure
# Mechanoreceptors:
* Contour following
#* Meissner's Corpuscles: Specialise in detecting fine precise touch and are concentrated in the fingertips
Therefore identifying a subjects properties<ref name="ref5"/>.
#* Pacinian Corpuscles: Respond to deep pressure and vibration regards sudden changes in pressure
#* Ruffini's Endings: Perceives sustained pressure and skin stretch, helping to maintain a firm grip on objects
 
At different rates of stimulation each of the receptor types adapts continuously.<ref>Andrade, Maria, et al. “The Senses 4: Touch – Physiology of the Sensation and Perception of Touch | Nursing Times.” ''Nursing Times'', 13 Dec. 2022, www.nursingtimes.net/neurology/the-senses-4-touch-physiology-of-the-sensation-and-perception-of-touch-13-12-2022/.</ref> Likewise it converts physical energy (pressure, vibration, temperature) into electrical impulses, which are send along nerve fibers towards the spinal cord ensuring instantaneous awareness of haptic changes.<ref>Blumenrath, Sandra . “The Neuroscience of Touch and Pain.” ''Www.brainfacts.org'', 3 Feb. 2020, www.brainfacts.org/thinking-sensing-and-behaving/touch/2020/the-neuroscience-of-touch-and-pain-013020.</ref> The electrical impulses are either transmitted through slowly adapting fibers, firing with persistent pressure, or rapidly adapting fibers. Once triggered, these signal travel to the somatosensory cortex, containing topographic maps of the skin, which represents different body parts.<ref name=":02" />
 
Furthermore, heating or cooling are detected by skin thermoreceptors influencing our recognition of objects (e.g metal feels colder than plastic at room temperature). If an object begins to warm up or cool down in the hand, through feedback humans can then indicate material properties such as moisture. The process from touching to interpretation for the haptic perception involves following steps:
 
# Input: Touch and vibration from the hands
# Kinesthetic Feedback: Awareness of finger and hand movement to judge weight and orientation
# Brain Processing: Integration into the cortex and other parietal regions
 
Therefore, the multiple lines of sensory information offer a detailed and adaptive representation of the physical world.<ref name=":02" />


====Perception of Sound====
====Perception of Sound====